All About Empathy

in Uncategorized
July 24th, 2010

Bookmark and Share

What contributes more to creating a person’s identity (i.e. personality, behavior, intelligence)? Is it genetics, or is it the environment in which the person was raised? In other words, as Francis Galton might ask, is it “nature” or “nurture?”

imageas

de Waal's book on empathy

When it comes to how empathetic someone is, Frans de Waal, a Dutch primatologist and ethologist, believes it’s both nature and nurture. He says that a person’s empathy is “innate” – inherited through genes – but also that a person can learn to become more or less empathetic. That seems reasonable; depending on early experiences and education, someone may be more or less of a certain characteristic.

But how is empathy innate? Two NewScientist writers, Philip Cohen and Ewen Callaway, wrote articles discussing the areas in our brains called the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the anterior insula (AI), which become active not only when we are in pain but also when others are.

Imaging studies, cited in their articles, found a positive correlation between a volunteer’s reported empathy for a person in pain and activity in the pain-processing areas of the volunteer’s brain. This has led Cohen to believe, “Humans are hardwired to feel empathy.”

For example, in a study led by Shihui Han and colleagues, “17 Chinese and 16 Caucasian (from the US, Europe and Israel) volunteers” were shown videos of strangers, both Caucasian and Chinese, in pain while their brains were scanned using fMRI. While their fMRI results suggested that they responded more empathetically towards volunteers of the same ethnicity or from the same country, their responses actually indicated they “[felt] each other’s pain about equally.”

Interestingly, our brains seem to be “hardwired” to feel more for certain groups over others, whether we notice or not. These groups appear to consist of people we can identify more with, whether through ethnicity, age, gender, or any other in-group.

Frans de Waal would find these results quite understandable. He says, “Empathy is more pronounced the more similar you are to someone, the more close, socially close, you are to someone.” He continues to say that empathy “evolved… for members of any species that is cooperative and social… it’s important to take care of others in the group because you depend on [them], you survive by [them].”

Seemingly then, our brains, and likely those of other species, have evolved to serve a survival advantage; they respond in those pain-processing areas more actively when those like us are in pain, despite what we report as our level of empathy.

While we seem to be hardwired to empathize more with certain groups over others, we’re still united as a species to empathize with one another over those of other species.

Martha Farah, a cognitive neuroscience researcher, suggests that we have a “person network” divided into persons and non-persons, which has promoted closer social bonds within our species. Farah proves that this brain network exists by considering the rare disorder prosopagnosia, which consists of “impaired visual recognition of the human face.” A specific area of the brain can be “selectively” damaged for one to obtain the disorder, demonstrating that specialized areas of the brain exist for discerning other humans.

Whether our brain also specializes in empathy towards non-persons is something to look into. For now, consider yawn contagion, which de Waal discusses with TIME about. He says there is a “deep bodily connection” that allows pets to catch yawns from their owners. This seemingly innate connection seems to break physical barriers with other animals, but what, if any, connection breaks emotional ones? And is it innate, or is it learned?

Have animal rights activists and pet lovers learned to be more empathetic towards non-persons? I’d like to think that it’s not just the influence of my environment that has led me to empathize with my childhood pets or toys – not to mention some of my favorite characters, like Hamm from Toy Story or Patrick from SpongeBob SquarePants.

Whether it is learned, innate, or both, I cannot say, but anthropomorphism seems to explain our emotional connections with non-humans. It breaks the barrier, allowing us to personify or add human characteristics to non-humans. For example, most people would probably like to think of their childhood pets as loved ones with human-like feelings and desires. However, would some stranger halfway across the world feel the same way you do about your pet? Probably not. They’d likely think of it as just another animal, simple as that.

chinchillaMost people, if asked if they support animal rights, would probably answer ‘Yes’ or some derivative1232_0064 of that. But, would they promise to never buy any animal-based products (eggs, meat, suede, leather, or even the chinchilla coat seen on Teresa last week in The Real Housewives of New Jersey)? Most likely not. I mean, for anyone, that’s a hard promise to keep when we have other priorities.

So how do we go from talking to our pets as if they were humans to absentmindedly buying products that might contain ingredients of an animal just like our pets?

de Waal says we do this through dehumanization. We go about anthropomorphizing our favorite pets, toys, and characters just as we go about dehumanizing them. By removing human characteristics, like emotion or spoken language, we don’t have to feel as bad about buying that leather jacket we always wanted. de Waal reminds us, “We eat nonhuman animals, wear them, perform painful experiments on them, hold them captive for purposes of our own – sometimes in unhealthy condition. We make them work, and we kill them at will.”

So, the next time you shop and find that animal-based product you just NEED to buy, take a second to think about how you’re setting your priorities. Think about how, maybe unconsciously or unintentionally, you are dehumanizing the animals used for the creation of the product you’re about to buy. Couldn’t that animal be from the same species as your favorite TV character, or even your old pet? I think so, easily.

Sources:

Are Humans Actually Selfish – Time

Brain’s response muted when we see other races in pain – NewScientist

Humans are hardwired to feel others’ pain – NewScientist

Primates and Philosphers: How Morality Evolved – Google books

Tagged , ,

6 Comments on All About Empathy

  • I wonder about the racial study…were those selected people from homogenous populations or those that were more or less isolated from diversity? Many of my friends are Asian. Over half my high school was too, so I find myself, even though I am white, almost being more comfortable around people of Chinese and Indian descent, for example, than white people. I would be inclined to believe that who we believe is similar to us might be based on proximity or familiarity, rather than physical comparison or innate sense of race.

  • I agree — the study as discussed on NewScientist doesn’t quite consider proximity or familiarity regarding empathy. I think, based on what I’ve read, particularly by de Waal, we are inclined to empathize more strongly with those we associate ourselves. So, whether you’re surrounded by those of Asian descent, those of the same gender, or those of any other in-group during your high school years or any other time, you’d likely understand those people better than those you didn’t surround yourself with, even if they’re from the same ethnicity or race, say Polish — assuming you have weaker ties with the Polish people or culture.

  • A study presented in a paper by Sangrigoli, et al. revealed that people of Korean descent who were adopted by Caucasian parents as children showed a reverse “other-race” effect, by demonstrating through performance in facial recognition tests that they more easily identified the Caucasian rather than the Asiatic faces, suggesting that the other-race effect is indeed purely due to nurture, rather than nature.
    It’s a good argument against segregation: it seems the more segregated a community is at first, the more segregated it will become.

  • Although these things have much to be criticized, television and the internet, (well globalized communication in general) may help to soften harsh demarcations people make about themselves and other races in their own mind though familiarity.

  • […] All About Empathy | the nerve blogJul 24, 2010 … But how is empathy innate? Two NewScientist writers, Philip Cohen and Ewen Callaway, wrote articles discussing the areas in our brains … […]

  • Great, just finished reading this blogpost. Very wonderful content you wrote. Surely following your site! Thank you very much.

Post Your Comment